Autonoetic consciousness




Autonoetic consciousness is the human ability to mentally place ourselves in the past, in the future, or in counterfactual situations, and to thus be able to examine our own thoughts.


Our sense of self affects our behavior, in the present, past and future. It relates to how we reflect on our own past behavior, how we feel about it, and this in turn determines if we do it again.[1][page needed]


It is episodic memory that deals with self-awareness, memories of the self and inward thoughts that may be projected onto future actions of an individual.[1][page needed] It was "proposed by [Endel] Tulving for self-awareness, allowing the rememberer to reflect on the contents of episodic memory".[2] Moreover, autonoetic consciousness involves behaviors such as mental time travel,[3][4] self-projection,[5] and episodic future thinking,[6] all of which have often been proposed as exclusively human capacities.[7]




Contents






  • 1 The self


  • 2 The parietal cortex


  • 3 Episodic memory and the self


  • 4 Event-related potentials


  • 5 Social anxiety disorder


  • 6 See also


  • 7 References


    • 7.1 Footnotes


    • 7.2 Bibliography




  • 8 Further reading





The self


Autonoetic consciousness is important in our formation of our "self" identity. What we have done in the past becomes a part of our "self" and the ability to reflect on this influences our behavior in the now.


In psychology, the self is often used for that set of attributes that a person attaches to himself or herself most firmly, the attributes that the person finds it difficult or impossible to imagine himself or herself without.[8][page needed] Identity is also used to describe this.[8] A person's gender is part of their identity but their profession, for example, may not be.[8][page needed]


In philosophy, the self is the agent, the knower and the ultimate locus of personal identity.[8][page needed] This self, the identity of which is at the bottom of every action, and involved in every bit of knowledge, is the self philosophers worry about.[8][page needed] Nevertheless, care of the self is of utmost importance in the bios-logos relationship.[9][full citation needed]


A straightforward view of the self would be that the self is just the person, and that a person is a physical system.[8][page needed] There are two problems with this view. First, the nature of freedom and consciousness has convinced many philosophers that there is a fundamentally non-physical aspect of persons.[8][page needed] The second challenge stems from puzzling aspects of self-knowledge, as the knowledge we have of ourselves seems very unlike the knowledge we have of other objects in several ways.[8][page needed]



The parietal cortex


The parietal cortex is strongly involved in autonoetic consciousness. Damage to areas of the parietal cortex can lead to different functioning errors, including changes in personality.


The parietal lobes can be divided into two functional regions. One involves sensation and perception, and the other is concerned with integrating sensory input, primarily with the visual system.[10] The first function integrates sensory information to form a single perception.[10] The second function constructs a spatial coordinate system to represent the world around us.[10]


Individuals with damage to the parietal lobes often show striking deficits, such as abnormalities in body image and spatial relations.[10]


Damage to the left parietal lobe can result in what is called Gerstmann's syndrome which includes right-left confusion, difficulty with writing, and difficulty with mathematics.[10] It can also produce disorders of language, and the inability to perceive objects normally.[10]


Damage to the right parietal lobe can result in neglecting part of the body or space, which can impair many self-care skills such as dressing and washing.[10] Right side damage can also cause difficulty in making things, denial of deficits, and drawing ability.[10]


Bi-lateral damage can cause Bálint's syndrome, a visual attention and motor syndrome.[10] This is characterized by the inability to voluntarily control the gaze, inability to integrate components of a visual scene, and the inability to accurately reach for an object with visual guidance.[10]


Left parietal-temporal lesions can affect verbal memory and the ability to recall strings of digits.[10]


The right parietal-temporal lobe is concerned with non-verbal memory.[10] Right parietal-temporal lesions can produce significant changes in personality.[10]


Lesions in the right parietal lobe influence personality, and this could be because the parietal lobe has to do with our sense of self. Our sense of self is strongly reflected in our personality.


Some common tests for parietal lobe function are: Kimura Box Test (apraxia) and the Two-Point Discrimination Test (somatosensory).[10]


During episodic retrieval, functional imaging studies consistently show differential activity in medial prefrontal and medial parietal cortices.[11]


With positron emission tomography, it has been shown that the medial regions are functionally connected and interact with lateral regions that are activated according to the degree of self-reference.[11]


For example, in one study, during retrieval of previous judgments of oneself, best friend, and the Danish Queen, activation increased in the left lateral temporal cortex and decreased in the right inferior parietal region with decreasing self-reference.[11] The decrease in parietal cortex activation may then prove it is a nodal structure in self representation, functionally connected to both the right parietal and the medial prefrontal cortices.[11] There was a decrease in the efficiency of retrieval of previous judgment of mental Self compared with retrieval of judgment of Other with transcranial magnetic stimulation at a latency of 160 ms, confirming the hypothesis that the medial parietal cortex in this network is essential for episodic memory retrieval with self-representation.[11]


This network is strikingly similar to the network of the resting conscious state, suggesting that self-monitoring is a core function in resting consciousness.[11]



Episodic memory and the self


For a coherent and meaningful life, conscious self-representation is mandatory.[11] Autonoetic consciousness is thought to emerge by retrieval of memory of personally experienced events (episodic memory).[11] Without the ability to reflect on our past experiences, we would be stuck in a state of constant awakening, without a past and therefore unable to prepare for the future.


Episodic memory is the memory we have for our past experiences, which influence our now, and our future. This is different from procedural memory, which is our memory for how to do things. Episodic memories influence our thinking about ourselves, good and bad.


Autobiographical memories can be retrieved from either the first person perspective, in which individuals see the event through their own eyes, or from the third person perspective, in which individuals see themselves and the event from the perspective of an external observer.[12]


A growing body of research suggests that the visual perspective from which a memory is retrieved has important implications for a person's thoughts, feelings, and goals, and is integrally related to a host of self- evaluative processes.[12]



Event-related potentials


Event-related potentials (ERPs) can measure autonoetic consciousness scientifically. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) are a non-invasive method of measuring brain activity during cognitive processing.[13] The transient electric potential shifts (so-called ERP components) are time-locked to the stimulus onset (e.g., the presentation of a word, a sound, or an image).[13] Each component reflects brain activation associated with one or more mental operations.[13]


In contrast to behavioral measures such as error rates and response times, ERPs are characterized by simultaneous multi-dimensional online measures of polarity (negative or positive potentials), amplitude, latency, and scalp distribution.[13] Therefore, ERPs can be used to distinguish and identify psychological and neural sub-processes involved in complex cognitive, motor, or perceptual tasks.[13]


Unlike fMRI, they provide extremely high time resolution, in the range of one millisecond.[13]


The methodological advantages of ERPs have resulted in an ever increasing number of ERP studies in cognitive neuroscience, cognitive psychology, psycholinguistics, neurolinguistics, neuropsychology, and neurology.[13] ERPs have also been used to identify patients who seem to be "brain-dead" but in fact are not.[13]


There is an event-related potential (ERP) experiment of human recognition memory that explored the relation between conscious awareness and electrophysiological activity of the brain.[14] ERPs were recorded from healthy adults while they made "remember" and "know" recognition judgments about previously seen words, reflecting "Autonoetic" and "Noetic" awareness, respectively.[14] The ERP effects differed between the two kinds of awareness while they were similar for "true" and "false" recognition.[14]


In a study of real-time noninvasive recordings of the brain's electrical activity (event-related potentials, ERPs), there was a common neural "signature" that is associated with self-referential processing regardless of whether subjects are retrieving general knowledge (noetic awareness) or re-experiencing past episodes (autonoetic awareness).[15]



Social anxiety disorder


Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is an example of how bad experiences can also lead to our behaviors. It demonstrates how our thoughts influence our feelings about ourselves and therefore our actions in society around us. It has to do with a person’s self-esteem, fear of failure, shame, fear of offending, and fear of strangers.


Cognitive models of social anxiety disorder believe the social self is a key psychological mechanism that maintains fear of negative evaluation in social and performance situations.[16][page needed] Consequently, a distorted self-view is evident when recalling painful autobiographical social memories, as reflected in linguistic expression, negative self-beliefs, and emotion and avoidance.[16][page needed]


To test this hypothesis, 42 adults diagnosed with SAD and 27 non-psychiatric healthy controls composed autobiographical narratives of distinct social anxiety related situations, generated negative self-beliefs, and provided emotion and avoidance ratings.[16][page needed]


Although narratives were matched for initial emotional intensity and present vividness, linguistic analyses demonstrated that, compared to the control group, the SAD group employed more self-referential, anxiety, and sensory words, and made fewer references to other people.[16][page needed] Social anxiety symptom severity, however, was associated with greater self-referential NSB in SAD only.[16][page needed]


SAD reported greater current self-conscious emotions when recalling autobiographical social situations, and greater active avoidance of similar situations than did the control group.[16][page needed] Autobiographical memory of social situations in SAD may influence current and future thinking, emotion, and behavioral avoidance.[16][page needed]



See also


  • Executive dysfunction


References



Footnotes





  1. ^ ab Baddeley, Eysenck & Anderson 2009.


  2. ^ Baddeley, Eysenck & Anderson 2015, p. 308.


  3. ^ Schacter, D. L.; Addis, D. R.; Buckner, R. L. (2007). "Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 8: 657–661..mw-parser-output cite.citation{font-style:inherit}.mw-parser-output .citation q{quotes:"""""""'""'"}.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-free a{background:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/6/65/Lock-green.svg/9px-Lock-green.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center}.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-limited a,.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-registration a{background:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/d/d6/Lock-gray-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-gray-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center}.mw-parser-output .citation .cs1-lock-subscription a{background:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/a/aa/Lock-red-alt-2.svg/9px-Lock-red-alt-2.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center}.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration{color:#555}.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription span,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration span{border-bottom:1px dotted;cursor:help}.mw-parser-output .cs1-ws-icon a{background:url("//upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/4/4c/Wikisource-logo.svg/12px-Wikisource-logo.svg.png")no-repeat;background-position:right .1em center}.mw-parser-output code.cs1-code{color:inherit;background:inherit;border:inherit;padding:inherit}.mw-parser-output .cs1-hidden-error{display:none;font-size:100%}.mw-parser-output .cs1-visible-error{font-size:100%}.mw-parser-output .cs1-maint{display:none;color:#33aa33;margin-left:0.3em}.mw-parser-output .cs1-subscription,.mw-parser-output .cs1-registration,.mw-parser-output .cs1-format{font-size:95%}.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-left,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-left{padding-left:0.2em}.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-right,.mw-parser-output .cs1-kern-wl-right{padding-right:0.2em} Cited in Hills & Butterfill 2015, p. 369.


  4. ^ Suddendorf, T.; Corballis, M. C. (2007). "The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30: 299–313. Cited in Hills & Butterfill 2015, p. 369.


  5. ^ Buckner, R. L.; Carroll, D. C. (2007). "Self-Projection and the Brain". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11: 49–57. Cited in Hills & Butterfill 2015, p. 369.


  6. ^ Atance, C. M.; O'Neill, D. K. (2001). "Episodic Future Thinking". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 5: 533–539. Cited in Hills & Butterfill 2015, p. 369.


  7. ^ Hills & Butterfill 2015, p. 369.


  8. ^ abcdefgh Perry, John (1995). "The Self" (PDF).
    [self-published source?]



  9. ^ Foucault, Parrhesia, 2007.


  10. ^ abcdefghijklmn "Parietal Lobes". TBI Resource Guide. Centre for Neuro Skills.
    [unreliable source?]



  11. ^ abcdefgh Lou et al. 2004, p. 6827.


  12. ^ ab Sutin & Robins 2008, p. 1386.


  13. ^ abcdefgh "Electroencephalography/Event Related Potentials (EEG/ERP) Laboratory". Brain and Language Lab. Washington: Georgetown University. Archived from the original on 19 July 2011. Retrieved 17 March 2018.
    [self-published source?]



  14. ^ abc Duzel et al. 1997, p. 5973.


  15. ^ Magno & Allan 2007, p. 672.


  16. ^ abcdefg Anderson et al. 2008.




Bibliography


.mw-parser-output .refbegin{font-size:90%;margin-bottom:0.5em}.mw-parser-output .refbegin-hanging-indents>ul{list-style-type:none;margin-left:0}.mw-parser-output .refbegin-hanging-indents>ul>li,.mw-parser-output .refbegin-hanging-indents>dl>dd{margin-left:0;padding-left:3.2em;text-indent:-3.2em;list-style:none}.mw-parser-output .refbegin-100{font-size:100%}



Anderson, Barrett; Goldin, Philippe R.; Kurita, Keiko; Gross, James J. (2008). "Self-Representation in Social Anxiety Disorder: Linguistic Analysis of Autobiographical Narratives". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 46 (10): 1119–1125. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.001. PMC 2630512. PMID 18722589.


Baddeley, Alan; Eysenck, Michael W.; Anderson, Michael C. (2009). Memory. New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84872-000-8.


 ———  (2015). Memory (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84872-183-8.


Duzel, Emrah; Yonelinas, Andrew P.; Mangun, George R.; Heinze, Hans-Jochen; Tulving, Endel (1997). "Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates of Two States of Conscious Awareness in Memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 94 (11): 5973–5978. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.11.5973. PMC 20891. PMID 9159185.


Hills, Thomas T.; Butterfill, Stephen (2015). "From Foraging to Autonoetic Consciousness: The Primal Self as a Consequence of Embodied Prospective Foraging" (PDF). Current Zoology. 61 (2): 368–381. Retrieved 17 March 2018.


Lou, Hans C.; Luber, Bruce; Crupain, Michael; Keenan, Julian P.; Nowak, Markus; Kjaer, Troels W.; Sackeim, Harold A.; Lisanby, Sarah H. (2004). "Parietal Cortex and Representation of the Mental Self". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 101 (17): 6827–6832. doi:10.1073/pnas.0400049101. PMC 404216. PMID 15096584.


Magno, Elena; Allan, Kevin (2007). "Self-Reference During Explicit Memory Retrieval: An Event-Related Potential Analysis". Psychological Science. 18 (8): 672–677. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01957.x. PMID 17680935.


Sutin, Angelina R.; Robins, Richard W. (2008). "When the 'I' Looks at the 'Me': Autobiographical Memory, Visual Perspective, and the Self". Consciousness and Cognition. 17 (4): 1386–1397. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.09.001. PMC 2733231. PMID 18848783.




Further reading






Gardiner, J. M. (2001). "Episodic Memory and Autonoetic Consciousness: A First-Person Approach". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 356 (1413): 1351–1361. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0955. PMC 1088519. PMID 11571027.


Libby, Lisa K. (2008). "A Neural Signature of the Current Self". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 3 (3): 192–194. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn031. PMC 2566766. PMID 19015110.









Popular posts from this blog

Shashamane

Carrot

Deprivation index